Personal-event memory

A personal-event memory is an individual's memory of an event from a certain moment of time. Its defining characteristics are that it is for a specific event; includes vivid multi-sensory elements (sights, sounds, smells, body positions, etc); is usually recalled in detail; and is usually believed by the individual to be an accurate representation of the event.[1]

Relation to other types of memory

Episodic memory and personal-event memories both are recollections of an event in one’s life. A personal-event memory is a more specific type of episodic memory in that there is more detail and sensory qualities to a personal-event memory.

Both flashbulb memory and personal-event memory contain highly vivid details and sensory qualities for a specific moment in time. They both also have a belief in accuracy of the event remembered. However, while flashbulb memories focus on events that are extremely striking and focus on the reception of shocking news, personal-event memories can include a wide variety of events that simply just stand out in the mind. A study (Talarico and Rubin, 2003) found a steady decrease in the accuracy and number of details remembered from a flashbulb memory after several months following the event.[2] Although this study found a decrease in recalled accuracy and number of from flashbulb memories, there is a similar study that explains that these memories are more resistant to fading than memories of other personal events at the time that the flashbulb memory was retained.[3]

Neurological components

Studies have shown that activity in the medial temporal lobe (MTL) structures:[4] perirhinal cortex, parahippocampal cortex and the hippocampus are vital to the recollection of personal-event memories[5] along with other memories. the medial temporal lobe structures, parietal and frontal lobes of the brain, all contribute to forming and retrieving memories. The MTL structures are the primary components for creating long-term memories via words, spatial information, and recognition.

The amygdala also has been linked to personal-event memory storage and retrieval. Laboratory studies have demonstrated more activity in the amygdala and hippocampus for recollection of emotional memories. The emotional component of these memories seems to play an important role in their ease of retrieval.[6]

Other studies demonstrate that Atrophy of the hippocampus due to aging in humans has been shown to decrease recollection of personal events.[7]

9/11 study

A study examined the memory of a group of Canadian students for the events of September 11, 2001 that took place in the United States. Pezdek hypothesized that whereas event memory would be better than autobiographical memory for a group that was personally involved with the event (i.e. people in New York where the attacks occurred), the reverse would be true for a group that was less involved, such as the Canadian sample. They found that memory for event information was less consistent than memory for autobiographical information. They also found that subjects recalled more information about the personal circumstances and feelings surrounding the event than they did about the actual event. Furthermore they found that the personal-event memory was significantly more accurate and more consistent in those participants who had experienced high arousal on hearing the news than in those who had experiences relatively little arousal. This better recall of the events of 9/11 for those who had experienced greater arousal supports Pezdek’s hypotheses that personal-event memory improves with higher levels of experienced emotion. Finally, they found that event memory is positively correlated with level of emotional arousal and shows significant decline with time.[8]

Event clusters

The study indicates that memorable personal events, regardless of age or importance, are often embedded in event clusters and that events organized by these clusters, like episodes in a story, are often causally related, temporally proximate, and similar in content.[9] It can also be argued that the clusters are created when people plan, execute, and evaluate meaningful action sequences and that[10] or narration serves to maintain or strengthen associations between clustered events.

Reminiscence bump

Main article: Reminiscence bump

Researchers have found that there seems to be a "reminiscence bump" where older adults recall more memories from their adolescence and early adulthood years. It seems to researchers that the memories of first-time or novel events during this time stand out in memory later in life.[11] In other words, memories of a first time personal experience stand out better in the mind than memories of repetition or routine. There are multiple hypotheses regarding the high occurrence of memory recall during adolescence and early adulthood; self-image hypothesis, cognitive hypothesis, and cultural life script hypothesis. the self-image hypothesis suggests that memory is heightened for events that happen while a young person is assuming a self-image or life identity.[12] The cognitive hypothesis proposes that memory encoding is better during the rapid changes that occur during adolescence and early adulthood.[13] The final explanation, cultural life script hypothesis, advocates the idea that there are culturally shared expectations that structure recall.[14]

References

  1. Pillemer DB (1998). Momentous Events, Vivid Memories. Harvard University Press. pp. 50–1. ISBN 978-0-674-58205-7.
  2. Talarico, Jennifer; Rubin, David (2003). "Confidence, not consistency, characterizes flashbulb memories". Psychological Science. 14 (5): 455–461. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.02453. PMID 12930476.
  3. Davidson, Patrick; Cook, Shaun; Glisky, Elizabeth (2002). "Flashbulb memories for September 11th can be preserved in older adults.". Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition. 13 (2): 196–206. doi:10.1080/13825580490904192.
  4. Davachi, Lila; Mitchell, J; Wagner, A (2003). "Multiple routes to memory: Distinct medial temporal lobes processes build item and source memories.". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 100 (4): 2157–2162. doi:10.1073/pnas.0337195100.
  5. Squire LR; Schacter DL (2003). Neuropsychology of Memory (3rd ed.). Guilford Press. pp. 6. ISBN 978-1-57230-898-5.
  6. Dolcos, F.; Labar, K. S.; Cabeza, R. (2005). "Remembering one year later: Role of the amygdala and the medial temporal lobe memory system in retrieving emotional memories". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 102 (7): 2626–2631. doi:10.1073/pnas.0409848102. PMC 548968Freely accessible. PMID 15703295.
  7. Schiltz, K.; Szentkuti, A.; Guderian, S.; Kaufmann, J.; Münte, T. F.; Heinze, H. J.; Düzel, E. (2006). "Relationship between Hippocampal Structure and Memory Function in Elderly Humans". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 18 (6): 990–1003. doi:10.1162/jocn.2006.18.6.990. PMID 16839305.
  8. Smith, M. C.; Bibi, U.; Sheard, D. E. (2003). "Evidence for the differential impact of time and emotion on personal and event memories for September 11, 2001". Applied Cognitive Psychology. 17 (9): 1047–1055. doi:10.1002/acp.981.
  9. Brown, N. R.; Schopflocher, D. (1998). "Event Clusters: An Organization of Personal Events in Autobiographical Memory". Psychological Science. 9 (6): 470–475. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00087.
  10. Memory rehearsal#Elaborative Rehearsal
  11. Janssen, S. M. J.; Murre, J. M. J. (2008). "Reminiscence bump in autobiographical memory: Unexplained by novelty, emotionality, valence, or importance of personal events". The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 61 (12): 1847–1860. doi:10.1080/17470210701774242. PMID 19031155.
  12. Rathbone, Clare; Moulin, Chris; Conway, Martin (2008). "Self-centered memories: The reminiscence bump and the self". Memory & Cognition. 38 (8): 1403–1414. doi:10.3758/mc.36.8.1403.
  13. Schrauf, Robert; Rubin, David (1998). "Bilingual Autobiographical Memory in Older Adult Immigrants: A Test of Cognitive Explanations of the Reminiscence Bump and the Linguistic Encoding of Memories". Journal of Memory and Language. 39 (3): 437–457. doi:10.1006/jmla.1998.2585.
  14. Berntsen, Dorthe; Rubin, David (2004). "Cultural life scripts structure recall from autobiographical memory". Memory & Cognition. 32 (3): 427–442. doi:10.3758/bf03195836.
This article is issued from Wikipedia - version of the 10/31/2016. The text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution/Share Alike but additional terms may apply for the media files.