Structure of Temperament Questionnaire
Structure of Temperament Questionnaire (STQ) is a test to measure 12 biologically and neurochemically based individual differences.
Purpose and format
The Structure of Temperament Questionnaire (STQ) is a self-report (for adult versions) or observer-administered (for children) questionnaire measuring 12 biologically-based characteristics of behavior. These characteristics are the most consistent aspects of behavior of an individual across his or her lifespan and are relatively independent of the content of the situation. Initially all versions of the STQ were validated on adult samples and were designed for the purposes of organizational, educational and clinical psychology. Now there are Child versions of the Short and Compact STQ, for administration by observers and guardians of the child in question. Items in all versions of the STQ are given in the form of a statement, with a response following the Likert scale format: "strongly disagree (1)," "disagree (2)," "agree (3)," "strongly agree (4)".
Experimental background of the STQ models
The STQ is based on the Eastern-European tradition of experiments investigating the types and properties of nervous systems. This tradition is the longest in (110 years old) among all traditions of temperamental research. It started from extensive experiments on several species of mammals, and then continued with human adults and children within the Pavlovian Institute of Highest Nervous Activity (Pavlov, 1941, 1957). It then was continued within the Laboratory of Differential Psychophysiology and Differential Psychology (Institute of Psychology of the Russian Academy of Sciences), supervised by Boris Teplov (1963), then Vladimir Nebylitsyn (1972), and then Vladimir Rusalov (1979). The STQ has several versions, which are based on two models of the structure of temperament: Rusalov`s model and Trofimova`s model. All versions of the STQ are based on the Activity-specific approach in temperament research. This approach differentiates between the traits related to 3 aspects of behavior: social-verbal, physical and mental. All models and all modern versions of the STQ have 12 temperament scales.
Rusalov`s versions of the STQ
There are two versions of the Structure of Temperament Questionnaire based on Rusalov’s model: an Extended STQ (STQ-150) and a Short STQ (STQ-26),[1][2] Both versions use 12 scales, which are grouped by 3 types of activities and 4 formal-dynamical aspects of activities, and a validity scale:
The Extended STQ is a 150-item self-report measure with 144 items assigned to 12 temperament scales (12 items each), 1 validity scale (6 items), and 6 indexes, which combine these scales. The values on each of temperament scales vary between 12 and 48. The validity scale is designed to measure a social desirability tendency. The value on this scale varies from 6 and 24, and protocols having a score higher than 17 on this scale are considered to be invalid.
Testing with the STQ-150 takes 30 minutes.
There was also an initial version of Rusalov`s model, STQ-105,[3] which used the same items and scales as the 8 scales of the STQ-150 (Extended) version, with the exception of the three scales related to intellectual aspects of activities (Intellectual Ergonicity, Intellectual Plasticity, Intellectual Tempo, Intellectual Emotionality). Rusalov upgraded his model to 12 (4 x 3) components implemented in his Extended STQ in the mid-1990-s.[1]
Rusalov`s version of the STQ measures 12 traits related to 4 aspects of behaviour (ergonicity (endurance), plasticity, tempo and emotionality), which are grouped by three aspects of behavior: motor-physical, social-verbal and intellectual. This model was incorporated in the extended Structure of Temperament Questionnaire. The factor analysis of the data received on Russian, Australian, American, Canadian, Urdu-Canadian, Polish-Canadian and Chinese samples confirmed a separation between the factors related to these three aspects of behavior.[1][2][4][5][6][7]
Administration of the Extended STQ in practice was rather time-consuming, so Rusalov and Trofimova agreed to develop shorter, more compact versions of the STQ, which would be more suitable for screening purposes in clinical, organizational, vocational and educational settings. The items with the highest item-total correlations were selected for these versions. Rusalov developed the Short version of the STQ, and Trofimova developed the Compact version of the STQ (STQ-77). The Short STQ (STQ-26) is composed of 2 out of 12 items on each scale of the Extended STQ, including the validity scale. This version was adapted for the assessment of adults, teenagers, preschool and early school children (Rusalov, 2004).
Trofimova`s (Compact) version of the STQ (STQ-77)
During the studies of psychometric properties of the Extended STQ and a selection of the most valid items for the Compact version of the STQ Trofimova suggested an alternative structure of temperament and reflected this structure in the Compact version of the STQ (STQ-77). The STQ-77 consists of 12 temperament scales (6 items each), and a validity scale (5 items), i.e. in total 77 items. STQ-77 has adult and several pilot Childhood versions.
Testing with the STQ-77 takes 12-15 minutes.
The STQ-77 arranges the dimensions of temperament into functional groups differently than the STQ-150 (compare two Figures). Similar to the Rusalov’s STQ-150, the STQ-77 differentiates between the traits regulating motor-physical, social-verbal and mental-probabilistic aspects of behaviour.[2][8][9] [10]
The differences between Trofimova’s and Rusalov’s models of temperament (and the structures of their versions of the STQ) are:
- the choice of grouping of temperament traits by 3 dynamical aspects (endurance, speed of integration of actions and orientation), presented as three columns in the Figure;
- a presence of orientation-related traits in Trofimova’s model which were not included in Rusalov’s model. These traits describe behavioural orientation of a person with preferences to specific types of reinforcers: sensations (Sensation Seeking), other people’s state (Empathy) or knowledge about causes of natural processes (a trait named as Sensitivity to Probabilities).
- a different structure of the traits related to emotionality. FET considers emotionality traits as systems amplifying three dynamical aspects of behaviour presented in the three columns of the model. Amplification of orientation aspects emerges in the trait of Neuroticism; amplification of speed of integration (i.e. immature integration) emerges as Impulsivity and amplification of subjective feeling of energetic capacities emerges in the trait of Self-confidence.
The STQ-77 is therefore partially based on the model of Rusalov’s STQ-150, but also on the work of Luria describing functionality of three neurophysiological systems: “sensory-informational block”, “programming block” and “energetic block” regulating human behaviour. The re-arrangement of the STQ scales in the STQ-77 scales was also based on the analysis of commonalities between the leading European and American models of temperament and the latest findings in neurophysiology and neurochemistry. The support of the STQ-77 architecture was recently reinforced by the review in neurochemistry research resulted in development of a neurochemical model Functional Ensemble of Temperament (FET) that maps an interplay between main neurotransmitter systems and temperament traits [11][12]
Description of the temperament scales of the STQ-77
- Mental Endurance, or Attention (ERI): the ability to stay focused on selected features of objects with suppression of behavioral reactivity to other features
- Motor-physical Endurance (ERM): the ability of an individual to sustain prolonged physical activity using well-defined behavioral elements
- Social-verbal Endurance (sociability, ERS): the ability of an individual to sustain prolonged social-verbal activities using well-defined behavioral elements
- Plasticity (PL): the ability to adapt quickly to changes in situations, to change the program of action, and to shift between different tasks
- Motor-physical Tempo (TMM): speed of integration of an action in physical manipulations with objects with well-defined scripts of actions
- Social-verbal Tempo (TMS): the preferred speed of speech and ability to understand fast speech on well-known topics, reading and sorting of known verbal material
- Sensitivity to Probabilities (PRO): the drive to gather information about uniqueness, frequency and values of objects/events, to differentiate their specific features, to project these features in future actions
- Sensation Seeking (SS): behavioral orientation to well-defined and existing sensational objects and events, underestimation of outcomes of risky behaviour
- Empathy (EMP): behavioral orientation to the emotional states/needs of others (ranging from empathic deafness in autism and schizophrenia disorders to social dependency)
- Neuroticism (NEU): A tendency to avoid novelty, unpredictable situations and uncertainty. Preference of well-known settings and people over unknown ones and a need for approval and feedback from people around.
- Impulsivity (IMP): Initiation of actions based on immediate emotional reactivity rather than by planning or rational reasoning
- Self-Confidence (SLF): A sense of security, dominance, self-esteem and in many cases entitlement for appreciation from others. In spite of the optimism about outcomes of his or her activities, the respondent might be negligent in details)
Versions in other languages and validation
Validation history of the STQ-105 and STQ-150
The Extended (STQ-150) version was adapted to five languages: English, Russian, Chinese, Polish and Urdu.[2]
Evidence for construct, concurrent and discriminatory validity of the STQ-105 and STQ-150 was demonstrated through significant correlations with the following measures:
- Pavlovian Temperamental Survey (PTS)
- Big Five Questionnaire (NEO-PI)
- Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (EPQ)
- Rogers adaptivity scale
- Taylor Manifest Anxiety Scale (MAS)
- Rosenzveig Test
- Motivation in Achievements Test
- Motivation of professional choices measure
- Liri Interpersonal Relations Test
- Cattell 16 Personality Factors Test (16PF)
- Wechsler IQ test
- Gotshield IQ test
- Shepard IQ test
- Speed of reading
- Auditory and visual sensitivity
- Alcohol impact
- Torrance Nonverbal Test of Creative Thinking
- Dembo-Hoppe Level of aspiration
- Dissociative Experiences Scale
- Rotter Locus of Control Scale (LOC)
- EEG data
- 25 measures of Mobility and Plasticity
- Verbal activity measures
Factor structure of the STQ-150
Factor analysis of the Russian version of the STQ-150 consistently showed four factors: Motor-physical activity (which includes Motor Ergonicity, Motor Plasticity, Motor Tempo), Social-Verbal activity (which includes Social Ergonicity, Social Plasticity, Social Tempo), Intellectual Activity (which includes Intellectual Ergonicity, Intellectual Plasticity, Intellectual Tempo) and Emotionality (3 scales of Emotionality) [1][2]
The administration of the English version of the STQ to American, Australian, and Canadian samples showed that the factor structure of this version is similar to the Russian language version, and that the English version possesses good reliability and internal consistency,[2][5][6][7][13] (Stough et al., 1991, Rusalov, 2004)
Chinese (STQ-C), Urdu (STQ-U) and Polish (STQ-P) Extended versions of the STQ, administered among corresponding populations, showed reliability coefficients in the range 0.70-0.86, item-total correlations in the range 0.42-0.73, and all versions demonstrated robust factor structures similar to those of the original version (Trofimova, 2010a).
Validation of the STQ-77
The Compact (STQ-77) version was adapted to three languages: English, Russian and Chinese.[2] There is work in progress on the Polish, Urdu, Hindu, Serbian, French, Dari and Korean versions of the STQ-77. Moreover, evidence for the construct, concurrent and discriminatory validity of the STQ-77 was demonstrated through significant correlations with the following measures:
- Rotter Locus of Control scale
- Five-Factors Personality test (NEO-FFI)
- high-school grades
- speed of performance in mental activities
- speed of verbal-semantic classification
- Pavlovian Temperamental Survey (PTS)
- Zuckerman Sensation Seeking Scale (SSS)
- I7 Impulsiveness Questionnaire (Eysenck, S. et al., 1985) (I-7)
- Hamilton Depression Inventory (HDI)
- State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI)
- Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI)
- Symptom Checklist (SCL-90)
- Verbal activity measures
- Personality Assessment Inventory
- Major Depression symptoms
- Generalized Anxiety symptoms
- symptoms of comorbid depression and anxiety
Clinical studies conducted with the use of the STQ-77 showed that its scales match the structure of symptoms of mental disorders described in main classifications (DSM-IV, ICD much better than other temperament models, and they are capable of differentiating between anxiety and depression [14]
Validation of the STQ-77 structure was reinforced by its comparison to the main findings in neurochemistry. As the result of such comparison the scales of the STQ-77 were linked to the ensemble interactions between main neurotransmitter systems (presented as a neurochemical Functional Ensemble of Temperament model) [11][12]
Factor structure of the STQ-77
The Confirmatory Factor Analysis of the Compact STQ (STQ-77) using data from a Canadian sample shows a satisfactory fit of the traditional 4-factor STQ activity-specific model, grouping the scales to the factors of Motor, Social, Intellectual activity and Emotionality and having 2 correlated residuals (from the new scale of Sensitivity to Sensations to Impulsivity and Neuroticism scales) with the CFI > .90, RMSEA < .07 and RMSR < .06.[9]
References
- 1 2 3 4 Rusalov, VM (1997). Oprosnik formal’no-dynamicheskih svoystv individual’nosti. [Questionnaire of formal-dynamical properties of individual. Moscow: IPRAN.
- 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Rusalov, VM; Trofimova, IN (2007). Structure of Temperament and Its Measurement. Toronto, Canada: Psychological Services Press.
- ↑ Rusalov, VM (1989). "Motor and communicative aspects of human temperament: a new questionnaire of the structure of temperament.". Personality and individual differences. 10: 817–827. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(89)90017-2.
- ↑ Trofimova, IN (2010). "Exploration of the activity-specific model of temperament in four languages". International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy. 10/1: 79–95.
- 1 2 Dumenci, L. (1996). "Factorial validity of scores on the Structure of Temperament Questionnaire.". Educational and Psychological Measurement. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. 56: 487–493. doi:10.1177/0013164496056003010.
- 1 2 Bishop, D.; et al. (1993). "Structure of Temperament Questionnaire (STQ): Results from a US sample". Personality and Individual Differences. 14: 485–487. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(93)90318-w.
- 1 2 Bishop, D.; Hertenstrein, M. (2004). "A confirmatory factor analysis of the Structure of Temperament Questionnaire". Educational and Psychological Measurement. 64: 1019–1029. doi:10.1177/0013164404264843.
- ↑ Trofimova, IN (2010). "Questioning the "general arousal" models". Open Behavioral Science and Psychology. 4: 1–8. doi:10.2174/1874230001004010001.
- 1 2 Trofimova, IN (2010). "An investigation into differences between the structure of temperament and the structure of personality". American Journal of Psychology. 123(4): 467–480. doi:10.5406/amerjpsyc.123.4.0467.
- ↑ Trofimova, IN; Sulis, W (2011). "Is temperament activity-specific? Validation of the Structure of Temperament Questionnaire – Compact (STQ-77)". International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy. 11(3): 389–400.
- 1 2 Trofimova, IN (2016). "The interlocking between functional aspects of activities and a neurochemical model of adult temperament.". In: Arnold, M.C. (Ed.) Temperaments: Individual Differences, Social and Environmental Influences and Impact on Quality of Life. New York: Nova Science Publishers, Inc.: 77–147.
- 1 2 Trofimova, I. & Robbins, T.W. (2016) Temperament and arousal systems: a new synthesis of differential psychology and functional neurochemistry. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 64, 382–402. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.03.008
- ↑ Trofimova (2009) Exploration of the benefits of an activity-specific test of temperament. Psychological Reports, 105, 643-658
- ↑ Trofimova, IN; Christiansen, J. (2016). "Coupling of temperament traits with mental illness in four age groups". Psychological Reports. 118(2): 387–412. doi:10.1177/0033294116639430. PMID 27154370.
Additional references (only STQ-related)
- Beere, D., & Pica, M. (1995) The predisposition to dissociate: the temperamental traits of flexibility/rigidity, daily rhythm, emotionality and interactional speed. Dissociation, VIII (4), 236-240.
- Bodunov M. V., Bezdenezhnykh B. N., & Alexandrov Yu. I. (1996) Charakteristika otvetov na testovie zadania psihologicheskih metodik i structura individual`nogo opita [Peculiarities of psychodiagnostic test item responses and the structure of individual experience].Psikhologicheskiy Zhurnal [Psychological Journal], 17(4), 87-96 [in Russian].
- Brebner, J., & Stough, C. (1993). The relationship between the Structure of Temperament and Extraversion and Neuroticism. Personality and Individual Differences. 14, 623-626.
- Byzova, V.M. (1997). Samoactualizacia predstaviteley Komi i russkogo etnosov [Self-actualization in representatives of Komi and Russian ethnic groups]. Psikhologicheskiy Zhurnal [Psychological Journal], 18(1), 61-69 [in Russian].
- Dumenci, L. (1995) The relation between the Structure of Temperament Questionnaire and other personality domains. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 55, 850-857. CA: Sage
- Dumenci, L. (1996) Factorial validity of scores on the Structure of Temperament Questionnaire. Educational and Psychological Measurement,56, 487-493. CA: Sage.
- Eputaev E. Y., Ikonnikova M. E., Agarkov B. A., & Tarabrina N. V. (2003) Dissociative states and formal-dynamical properties of individual. Materials of 7th Multidisciplinary Conference `Stress And Behavior`, 121-123. Moscow: IPRAN Press.
- Eysenck, H. J. & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1968) Manual for the Eysenck Personality Inventory. San Diego, CA: Educational and Industrial Testing Service.
- Gray, J. A (1964) Pavlov`s typology; recent theoretical and experimental developments from the laboratory of B. M. Teplov. New York: Macmillan.
- Gray, J. A. (1982). The neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry into the functions of the septo-hippocampal system. Oxford University Press.
- Nebylitsyn, V. D. (1972) Fundamental properties of the human nervous system. New York: Plenum.
- Pavlov, I. P. (1941) Lectures on conditioned reflexes, v. IIi: types of the higher nervous activity, their interdependence with neuroses and psychoses and the physiological mechanism of neurotic and psychotic symptoms. NY: International Publishers.
- Pavlov, I. P. (1957) General Types of Animal and Human Higher Nervous Activity. Experimental Psychology and other Essays. Philosophical Library, Inc., New York.
- Postman, L.., Bruner, J. S., & McGinnies, E. M. (1946) Personal values as selective. factors in perception. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 43, 142-154
- Popov Yu. A. (2006) Trevojnost` v psycho-pedagogocheskom portrete studenta kolledga [Anxiety in psycho-pedagogical portrait of a College student]. Professional`noye obuchenie [Professional Education]. 8, 24-25. Moscow, Russia. [in Russian]
- Rathee N., & Singh, R. (2001) Mobility or/and Lability of the Nervous Processes as Temperamental Trait(s). Personality and Individual Differences, 31, 1091-1104.
- Ruch, W., Angleitner, A., & Strelau, J. (1991) The Strelau Temperament Inventory Revised (STI-R): Validity studies. European Journal of Personality, 5, 287-308.
- Rusalov, V. M. (2004) Formal`no-dynamicheskiye svoystva individual`nosti (temperament) [Formal-dynamical properties of individual (temperament)]. Russian Academy of Sciences, IPAN Press: Moscow. [in Russian]
- Rusalov, V. M., & Dudin C. I. (1995) Temperament i intellect: obchie b specificheskiye factori rasvitiya [Temperament and Intelligence: general and specific factors of development]. Psikhologicheskiy Zhurnal [Psychological Journal], 16 (5), 12-23. [in Russian]
- Rusalov, V. M., & Kalashnikov, S. V. (1988) On the correlation of psychic plasticity with the integral factors of human brain bioelectrical activity. In: Rusalov, V. M. (Ed.) Individual`no -psikhologicheskie razlichiya i bioelektricheskaya aktivnost` mozga cheloveka (Individual Psychological Differences and Human Brain Bioelectrical Activity), 5-55. Moscow: Nauka.
- Rusalov, V. M., & Kalashnikova I. G. (1992) Psychological testing of temperament. Zhurnal Vysshey Nervnoy Deyatel`nosti (Journal of Higher Nervous Activity), 42(1), 44.
- Rusalov, V. M., & Naumova E. R. (1999) On the relationship between general abilities and "intellectual" scales of temperament.Psikhologicheskiy Zhurnal [Psychological Journal], 20 (1), 70-77
- Rusalov, V. M., & Poltavtzeva, L. I. (1997) Temperament as a prerequisite of creative abilities. Pavlov Journal of Higher Nervous Activity, 47, 3, 451-460.
- Rusalov, V. M, Rusalova, M. N., & Strel`nikova, E. V. (2000) Temperament cheloveka i osobennosti vibora mejdu veroyatnostyu dostijeniya tseli i ee tzennostyu [Temperament of man and peculiarities of choice between the probability of goal achievement and its value. Zhurnal Vysshey Nervnoy Deyatel`nosti [Journal of Higher Nervous Activity], 50(3), 388. [in Russian]
- Rusalov, V. M., Trofimova, I. N. (2011). О представленности типов психической деятельности в различных моделях темперамента.Психологический журнал, 32, 3, 74-84.
- Stough, C., Brebner, J., & Cooper, C. (1991) The Rusalov Опросник Структуры Темперамента (STQ): results from an Australian sample.Personality and Individual Differences, 12, 1355-1357.
- Strelau, J. (1999) The Pavlovian Temperament Survey (PTS): An international handbook. Germany: Hogrefe, & Huber.
- Strelau, J., & Angleitner, A. (Eds.) (1991) Explorations in temperament: International perspectives on theory and measurement. N.Y.: Plenum.
- Teplov, B.M. & Nebylitsyn, V.D. (1963) Experimental study of properties of the nervous system in man. Journal of Highest Nervous Activity, 13: 789-797.
- Trofimova I. (1999). How people of different age, sex and temperament estimate the world. Psychological Reports. N 85/2, pp. 533–552.
- Trofimova I.N. (1997) Interconnections of characteristics of temperament with some peculiarities of cognitive activity of human // Questions of psychology, N 1. pp. 74–82. .
- Trofimova, I. (2014) Observer bias: how temperament matters in semantic perception of lexical material. PLOS ONE, 9(1): e85677. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0085677
- Vasyura, S. A. (2008) Psychology of male and female communicative activity. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 11, 289-300.
- Vorobieva E. V. (2004) Modern psychogenetic studies of intelligence and theory of motivation for achievements. Journal of Applied Psychology. 3, 53-59.
- Watkins D., Mortazavi S. & Trofimova I. (2000). Independent and interdependent conceptions of self: an investigation of age, gender and culture differences in importance and satisfaction ratings. Cross Cultural Research. Vol. 34, N2, pp. 113–134.
- Zin`ko E. V. (2006) Kharakteristiki samootcenki i urovnya dostijeniy i ih sopolojenie [Characteristics of self-image and of level of aspiration and its relationships]. Psikhologicheskiy Zhurnal [Psychological Journal], 3, 18-30; 4, 15-25.